ISSN: 2755-1067 | Open Access

Journal of Diseases Disorders & Treatments

Digit Ratio (2d:4d) in Patients with Antisocial Personality Disorder

Author(s): Osman Mermi*, Sevler Yildiz, Mustafa Nuray Namli and Murad Atmaca

Abstract

Objective: The present study aimed to compare the 2nd finger to 4th finger ratio (2D:4D) of patients with antisocial personality disorder to the 2D:4D of a group of healthy subjects.

Method: A total of thirty one male patients with antisocial personality disorder and the twenty-eight healthy controls were included in the present investigation. Finger (2D) and ring (4D) lengths and 2D:4D ratio of the subjects were determined.

Results: In the present study, we found that patients with antisocial personality disorder had a significantly lower ratio of 2D:4D of both hands.

Conclusion: In conclusion, we suggest that patients with antisocial personality seem to have a lower ratio of 2D:4D compared to healthy control subjects, leading us to think that higher prenatal gonadal androgens may be related to antisocial personality disorder.

Introduction

An antisocial personality disorder is one of the personality disorders that is placed in B cluster personality disorders in Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5 (DSM 5). It is characterized by a pervasive pattern of disregard for the rights of other people that manifests as hostility and/or aggression [1]. On the other hand, manipulation and deceit are other important clinical characteristics. In general, its clinical features start to appear in the childhood period of their life. It is accepted as a life-long condition, with a prevalence of 1-3% of the general population and 40-70% of the people staying in prisonsn. As in other personality disorders, little is known about the occurrence of antisocial personality disorder, as much it is well-known that genetic factors are important. On the other hand, it has been proposed that antisocial behaviors might be associated with alterations in different brain regions [2-4].

The digit ratio [the 2nd digit (index finger) and 4th digit (ring finger), 2D:4D] has been described as the ratio of the lengths of the second and fourth digits of the hand. Recently, the relative length difference between 2D and 4D has been the subject of the various psychiatric conditions. Different investigations have emphasized the relationship of a variety of psychological traits and psychiatric disorders to the 2D:4D. 2D:4D is a sexually dimorphic feature. It has been linked 2D:4D to prenatal testosterone and estrogen, suggesting that the ratio of 2D:4D might be affected by higher prenatal testosterone levels or greater sensitivity to androgen. Because of this effect, it is believed the ratio of 2D:4D to be established in the early development period in males, there has been a relatively shorter 2nd finger than 4th finger, while females have 2nd and 4th digits of equal lengths or a longer 2nd finger compared to 4th finger. So, the 2D:4D of the males is generally lower than that of the females. On the other hand, it has been shown that this lower is obvious on the right side compared to the left side of the hands [5-12].

As much there have been contrary knowledge, prenatal exposure to androgens has been emphasized as a determinant of aggressive behavior in later life. In males compared to females, life-time persistent antisocial behaviors are reported to be over ten times. This relationship led the investigators to consider that male gonadal hormones such as testosterone, might play an important role in the occurrence of antisocial behaviors. Antisocial personality disorder and conduct disorder are both associated with higher levels of testosterone. On the other hand, Dabbs and Morris reported that army veterans with the presence of conduct disorder symptoms during childhood period to have 1.4 times more likely in high free testosterone subjects compared to low free testosterone subjects. Additionally, some investigations have determined that there have been significant positive correlations between conduct disorder symptoms which were retrospectively described, and total testosterone values in childhood and adulthood [13-29].

Low 2D:4D which corresponds to masculine and androgenic type has been reported to be correlated with greater values of aggression and sensation seeking, and physically aggression in males [7,8].In the present study, our hypothesis was that patients with antisocial personality disorder would have a lower 2D:4D compared to that of healthy control subjects.

Methods

We measured 2D:4D in the right and left hands of thirty one patients who had applied to our in-patient and out-patient clinics at Firat University School of Medicine Department of Psychiatry, Elazig, Turkey. All patients were diagnosed with an antisocial personality disorder by a senior psychiatry assistant by using the DSM 5. Edition criteria. Based on their answers to the question: “what is your sexual orientation?”, all patients had a heterosexual orientation. We took the approval of the Local Ethics Committee at the Firat University School of Medicine. After this approval, from all subjects, written informed consent was obtained to participate in the study. We used some exclusion criteria: being under eighteen years old, the presence of congenital anomaly consisting of bones, and the existence of acromegaly. On the other hand, the same number of healthy control subjects were included in the study. They were age and sex-matched ones. Some criteria were also administered to the healthy control subjects. Healthy control subjects were in healthy physical and mental status. First of all, those who had any current or previous history of neurological or psychiatric conditions, endocrinologic abnormalities, and had congenital anomalies consisting of bones, and acromegaly were not included in the investigation.

Digit Ratio (2D:4D) Procedure

For all patients with antisocial personality disorder and healthy control subjects, it was photographed the left and right-hand palms by keeping the camera perpendicular to the palms side in the same height. When we took the image of the hand, the participants were asked to spread their fingers and hand palms as possible. These images were utilized to determine the digit length and ratio of the subjects. The total lengths of the second and fourth digits were quantified from the middle of the basal crease to the fingertips. Each of the second and fourth fingers was measured three times by two independent raters who were blind to the subjects’ group. We took the average values for each finger after the measurements. After this process, we determined the ratios of digit lengths between index finger and ring finger and accepted the average digit ratio value of two measurements as the final result. All measurements were performed by digital sensitive calipers, with the sensitivity of the nearest 0.01mm. The unit of the finger length presented in Table 1 was in centimeters.

Table 1: Demographic, clinical and digit measurement data

Antisocial case group (n=31) Healty control group (n=28) p value
Age Mean ± SD 33.29 ± 8.42 34.17 ± 8.81 0.695
Gender Male 27 (87.1 %) 18 (64.3 %) 0.040
Female 4 (12.9 %) 10 (35.7 %)
Marital status Married 19 (61.3 %) 20 (71.4 %) 0.411
Single 12 (38.7 %) 8 (28.6 %)
Education status middle school and below 23 (74.2 %) 10 (35.7 %) 0.003
high school and above 8 (25.8 %) 18 (64.3 %)
Economical status Lower 22 (71 %) 6 (21.4 %) <0.001
Middle 9 (29 %) 20 (71.4 %)
Upper 0 2 (7.1 %)
Working status Employed 17 (54.8 %) 24 (85.7 %) 0.010
Unemployed 14 (45.2 %) 4 (14.3 %)
suicide attempts Yes 8 (25.8 %) 0 0.005
No 23 (74.2 %) 28 (100 %)
Dominant hand Right 29 (93.5 %) 24 (85.7 %) 0.409
Left 2 (6,5 %) 4 (14,3 %)
Tobacco use Yes 18 (58.1 %) 14 (50 %) 0.535
No 13 (41.9 %) 14 (50 %)
Alcohol use Yes 12 (38.7 %) 0 <0.001
No 19 (61.3 %) 28 (100 %)
Self-mutilation Yes 11(35.5 %) 0 <0.001
No 20 (64.5 %) 28 (100 %)
Forensic event Yes 8 (25.8 %) 0 0.005
No 23 (74.2 %) 28 (100 %)
Presence of psychiatric illness in the family Yes 10 (32.3 %) 7 (25 %) 0.539
No 21 (67.7 %) 21(75 %)
2D/4D ratio Left hand (Mean ± SD) 0.965 ± 0.011 0.979 ± 0.026 0.021
Right hand (Mean ± SD) 0.967 ± 0.011 0.978 ± 0.025 0.041

The finger length unit presented in Table 1 is centimetre.

Statistical Analysis

SPSS v16.0 software was used for statistical analysi (SPSS Inc., Chicago). For continuous variables and finger lengths, it was used independent sample t-tests. On the other hand, when required, for categorical variables, a Pearson chi-square test was utilized. Correlation relationships were analyzed by using Pearson correlation coefficient. The p-value <0.05 was regarded as having a significant difference. For categorical variables Fisher exact test was used.

Results

First of all, we did not detect any statistically significant difference in terms of some demographic and clinical variables while detecting for some others, as shown in Table 1. Reliability of the two raters was high for both the right hand (2D: ICC=0.99; 4D: ICC=0.99; 2D:4D: ICC=0.98) and the left hand (2D: ICC=0.98; 4D: ICC=0.99; 2D:4D: ICC=0.98).

We found that the mean digit ratio of 2D:4D for the left hand of patients with antisocial personality disorder and healthy control subjects were 0.965±0.011 and 0.979±0.026, respectively. The mean digit ratio of 2D:4D of the patient group was statistically significantly different from that of healthy comparisons (p<0.05). On the other hand, we observed that the mean digit ratio of 2D:4D for the right hand of patients with antisocial personality disorder and healthy control subjects were 0.967±0.011 and 0.978±0.025, respectively. Likewise, the mean digit ratio of 2D:4D of the patient group was statistically significantly different from that of healthy comparisons (p<0.05).

When performed on correlation analyses, we did not observe any correlational associations between any demographic variables and the ratio of 2D:4D for both sides of hands and scale scores for both study groups of patients with antisocial personality disorder and healthy controls (p>0.05).

Discussion

In the present study, we determined that both sides of ratios of 2D:4D were statistically significantly lower than those of healthy comparison subjects, without any correlation between any demographic variables and the ratio of 2D:4D for both sides of hands and scale scores for both study groups of patients with an antisocial personality disorder.

As in our previous unpublished study in which has been performed on patients with a borderline personality disorder, we think that because the ratio of 2D:4D might be conversely associated with the prenatal exposure to androgens, our present results on patients with a borderline personality disorder can show that increased androgen exposure during the fetal period might be raising the risk for the occurrence of antisocial personality disorder. Our results are in accordance with the study of Martel et al.’ who revealed that lower 2D:4D is related to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in male patients but not in female ones [30]. Recent years, in various medical and psychiatric conditions, the ratio of 2D:4D has been measured. Concerning psychiatric disorders, especially aggression and impulsivity related conditions, the ratio has been taken attention. Because prenatal exposure to androgens has been proposed to be linked to aggressive behaviors in the later period of life [13-16]. In association with this, in patients with conduct disorder, the ratio of 2D:4D has been evaluated [31]. Because there has been reported an association between aggressive and impulsive behaviors and the ratio of 2D:4D, our study team examined the ratio of 2D:4D in patients with borderline personality disorder and healthy control subjects comparatively in a cross-sectional designed study and detected that patients with borderline personality disorder had lower 2D:4D ratio compared to healthy control subjects, suggesting that higher prenatal androgenic exposure during the fetal period may be associated with a borderline personality disorder. Antisocial personality and borderline personality disorders are both placed in the same category of personality disorders, in the B cluster, in the DSM 5. They have some similar clinical characteristics such as aggression, and impulsive behaviors. Moving from this point, our similar results with both borderline and antisocial personality disorders which were the lower ratio of 2D:4D led us to consider that patients with an antisocial personality disorder might have exposure to raised androgenic hormones during the fetal period of life compared to healthy control comparisons, as speculated in our previous another study on patients with borderline personality disorder (Atmaca et al., unpublished study).

Our present study has some similar limitations, as in our previous study on patients with borderline personality disorder (Atmaca et al., unpublished study). First one, the sample size of the study was small for such types of investigations. Second one, this type of studies includes sensitive measurements, individual differences to measure may influence our findings. However, as mentioned in the Methods section, each of the second and fourth fingers was measured three times by two independent raters who were blind to the subjects’ group and wee took the average values for each finger after the measurements, and finally, we determined the ratios of digit lengths between the index finger and ring finger and accepted the average digit ratio value of two measurements as the final result.

Conclusion

In Conclusion, we suggest that patients with antisocial personality seem to have a lower ratio of 2D:4D compared to healthy control subjects, leading us to think that higher prenatal gonadal androgens may be related to antisocial personality disorder.

Conflict of Interests

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest relating to this study or to this publication.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Mental Disorders (Dsm 5). In: American Psychiatric Association.
  2. Lenzenweger MF, Lane MC, Loranger AW, Kessler RC (2007) DSM-IV Personality Disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication, Biol Psychiatry 62: 553-564.
  3. Muldoon MF, Manuck SB, Matthews KA (1990) Lowering cholesterol concentrations and mortality: A quantitative review of primary prevention trials, Br Med J 301: 309-314.
  4. Driessen M, Herrmann J, Stahl K, Zwaan M, Meier S, et al. (2000) Magnetic Resonance Imaging Volumes of the Hippocampus and the Amygdala in Women With Borderline Personality Disorder and Early Traumatization. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57: 1115-1122.
  5. Brown WM, Finn CJ, Breedlove SM (2002) Sexual dimorphism in digit-length ratios of laboratory mice, Anat Rec 267: 231-234.
  6. Ökten A, Kalyoncu M, Yariş N (2002) The ratio of secondand fourth-digit lengths and congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Early Hum Dev 70: 47-54.
  7. Manning JT, Scutt D, Wilson J, Lewis-Jones DI (1998) The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone andoestrogen. Hum Repro 13: 3000-3004.
  8. Brown WM, Hines M, Fane BA, Breedlove SM (2002) Masculinized Finger Length Patterns in Human Males and Females with Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia, Horm Behav. 42: 380-386.
  9. Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Knickmeyer R, Manning JT (2004) 2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol, Early Hum Dev. 77: 23-238.
  10. Manning JT, Barley L, Walton J, Lewis-Jones DI, Trivers RL, et al. (2000) The 2nd:4th digit ratio, sexual dimorphism, population differences, and reproductive success: evidence for sexually antagonistic genes? Evol Hum Behav. 21: 163-183.
  11. McFadden D, Shubel E, (2002) Relative Lengths of Fingers and Toes in Human Males and Females, Horm Behav. 42: 492-500.
  12. Williams TJ, Pepitone ME, Christensen SE, Cooke BM, Huberman AD, et al. (2000) Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation, Nature. 404: 455-456.
  13. Bailey AA, Hurd PL (2005) Finger length ratio (2D:4D) correlates with physical aggression in men but not in women, Biol Psychol. 68: 215-222.
  14. Joyce CW, Kelly JC, Chan JC, Colgan G, O Briain D (et al). (2013) Second to fourth digit ratio confirms aggressive tendencies in patients with boxers fractures, Injury. 44: 1636- 1639.
  15. Kilduff LP, Hopp RN, Cook CJ, Crewther BT, Manning JT (2013) Digit Ratio (2D:4D), Aggression, and Testosterone in Men Exposed to an Aggressive Video Stimulus, Evol Psychol. 1: 11.
  16. Voracek M, Stieger S (2009) Replicated nil associations of digit ratio (2D:4D) and absolute finger lengths with implicit and explicit measures of aggression, Psicothema. 21: 382-389.
  17. Moffit TE, Caspi A, Rutter M, Silva PA (2001) Sex differences in antisocial behavior, Conduct disorder, delinquency and violence in the Dunedin Longitudinal Study, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press p, 278.
  18. Moffit TE, Caspi A (2003) Life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial behavior: A 10-year research review and a research agenda, In: Moffit TE, Caspi A, editor, Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency, New York: The Guilford Press. p: 49-75.
  19. Moffitt TE (2006) Life-course-persistent versus adolescentlimited antisocial behavior, Ciccheti D, Cohen D, editor, 2nd Ed, Development psychopathology, New York: John Wiley and Sons. 570-598 p.
  20. Eme R (2009) Male life-course persistent antisocial behavior, A review of neurodevelopmental factors, Aggress Violent Behav. 14: 348-358.
  21. Van Honk J, JLG Schutter D (2007) Testosterone Reduces Conscious Detection of Signals Serving Social Correction: Implications for Antisocial Behavior, Psychol Sci. 1:18: 663- 667.
  22. Virkkunen M, Linnoila M, Brain serotonin (1993) type II alcoholism and impulsive violence, J Stud Alcohol, Suppl. 1: 163-169.
  23. . Aromäki AS, Lindman RE, Eriksson CJP (1999) Testosterone, aggressiveness, and antisocial Personality, Aggress Behav. 1:25: 113-123.
  24. Aromäki AS, Lindman RE, Eriksson CJP(1999) Testosterone, sexuality and antisocial personality in rapists and child molesters: a pilot study, Psychiatry Res. 110: 239-247.
  25. Chance SE, Brown RT, Dabbs JM, Casey R (2000) Testosterone, intelligence and behavior disorders in young boys, Pers Individ Dif. 28: 437-445.
  26. Glenn AL, Raine A, Schug RA, Gao Y, Granger DA (2011)
  27. Increased testosterone-to- cortisol ratio in psychopathy, Vol, 120, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Glenn, Andrea L: 3720 Walnut Street, Philadelphia PA, US, American Psychological Association : 389-399.
  28. Dabbs JM1992 Testosterone and Occupational Achievement, Soc Forces [Internet] Mar. 10:70: 813-824
  29. Booth A, Osgood DW (1993) The influence of testosterone on deviance in adulthood: assessing and explaining the relationship, Criminology. 1:31: 93-117.
  30. Mazur A (1995) Biosocial models of deviant behavior among male army veterans Biol Psychol. 41: 271-293.
  31. Martel MM, Gobrogge KL, Breedlove SM, Nigg JT (2008) Masculinized finger-length ratios of boys, but not girls, are associated with attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder, Vol 122, Behavioral Neuroscience, Martel, Michelle M, Michigan State University, 43 Psychology Building, East Lansing MI, US, 48823-1116, American Psychological Association: P, 273-281.
  32. Eichler A, Heinrich H, Moll GH, Beckmann MW, Goecke TW, Fasching PA et al. (2018) Digit ratio (2D:4D) and behavioral symptoms in primary-school aged boys, Early Hum Dev.119: 1-7.
View PDF